http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cya.2017.06.008
Paper Research
Material values: A study of some antecedents and
consequences
Valores
materiales: un estudio de algunos antecedentes y consecuencias
Rogelio Puente Díaz1
Judith Cavazos Arroyo2
1 Universidad
Anáhuac, Mexico
2 Universidad
Popular Autónoma del Estado de Puebla, Mexico
Corresponding author: Rogelio Puente Díaza,
email: rogelio.puente@anahuac.mx
Abstract
Two studies were
conducted to analyze some antecedents and consequences of holding material
values among college students from Mexico. Participants completed a battery of
questionnaires measuring social comparison orientation, autonomy support,
materialism, life satisfaction, positive and negative affect, and job
preferences. Results showed that social comparison was a robust predictor of
materialism in both studies. Materialism was related to lower levels of
positive affect and life satisfaction and to higher levels of negative affect.
Materialism was also related to a preference for jobs that pay more money at
the expense of working longer hours or doing more routine tasks. The
implications of the results were discussed.
Keywords: Material values, Consumer behavior, Subjective
well-being.
JEL classification: M31, I31, J00.
Resumen
Se realizaron dos estudios para analizar algunos
antecedentes y consecuencias de la presencia de valores materiales entre
estudiantes universitarios en México. Los participantes completaron una batería
de cuestionarios que miden orientación a la comparación social, apoyo a la
autonomía, materialismo, satisfacción con la vida, afecto positivo y negativo,
y preferencias laborales. Los resultados mostraron que la comparación social
fue un predictor robusto del materialismo en ambos estudios. El materialismo se
relacionó con los niveles más bajos de afecto positivo y satisfacción con la
vida y con los niveles más altos de afecto negativo. El materialismo también se
relacionó con una preferencia por los trabajos que pagan más dinero a costa de
trabajar más horas o hacer las tareas más rutinarias. Las implicaciones de los
resultados fueron discutidas.
Palabras clave: Valores
materiales, Comportamiento del consumidor, Bienestar subjetivo.
Códigos JEL: M31, I31, J00.
Material values: an exploration of
some antecedents and consequences
Values play a central
role since they influence the goals and life styles people choose and the
emotions they feel ( Schwartz, 2005 ). Values also play an important
role in consumer research ( Pepper, Jackson, &
Uzzell, 2009 ). Specifically, investigators,
interested in understanding the social psychology of consumer behavior, have
turned their attention to the examination of some antecedents and consequences
of holding material values. Examining the antecedents and consequences of
holding material values seems particularly relevant given the economic
uncertain times we are living in. Consequently, the present investigation,
using the theory of material values as guiding framework ( Kasser, 2002 ), attempts to make a
contribution by: (1) examining the role of social comparison orientation, and
autonomy support as antecedents of material values and also (2) assessing the
affective, cognitive, and behavioral consequences of holding material values.
In order to accomplish this goal, we first review the relevant literature on
values.
Material values
Even though there are
different values that individuals might hold, material values seem particularly
relevant for understanding consumer behavior ( Grougiou & Moschis, 2015 ). Material values give importance
to the acquisition of material goods and possessions ( Richins & Dawson, 1992) and consumer style (Zampieri et al., 2012 ). The theory of
materialistic values ( Kasser, 2002 ) seeks to answer two
important questions: (1) what drives individuals to value material possessions
and (2) what are some of the consequences of holding material values. Even
though consumer researchers ( Kasser, 2002; Richins & Dawson, 1992 ) have warned us about
the widespread endorsement of material values, we would expect consumers to
have significant variation in their levels of endorsement of material values
and this is precisely one of the things that we would like to explain. That is,
we would like to identify which variables act as antecedents of materialism.
Antecedents: social
comparison and autonomy support
The theory of
materialistic values (Kasser, 2002 ) suggests that
individuals might come to endorse material values for two main reasons: (1)
social models endorsing the importance of material possessions, a socialization
process and (2) lack of fulfillment of the basic psychological needs for
autonomy, relatedness and competence, which creates feelings of insecurity.
Both factors can act as independent antecedents or in conjunction. In the
present investigation, we analyze the role of social comparison orientation
(study 1 and 2) and autonomy support (study 2) as proxies of the two factors
suggested by the theory of material values and examine their role as possible
antecedents of materialism.
According to social comparison
theory ( Festinger, 1954 ), people have the
drive to evaluate themselves (self-evaluation) by comparing their opinions and
abilities with others. Social comparison is such a common and central process
that can occur automatically, without conscious intent ( Mussweiler, 2007 ). Our argument is that
consumers also use social comparisons to learn and evaluate what is desirable
in terms of material possessions. Consumer researchers extend social comparison
theory to include the comparison of material possessions, suggesting that material
possessions provide individuals with a source of information about what is
valuable and desirable and also help communicate their social standing in
relation to others. Thus, individuals might be prone to not only compare their
opinions and abilities with others, but also their material possessions.
With regard to the role
of social comparison in learning what material possessions are desirable, Duesenberry (1949) proposes the term
demonstration effect. Demonstration effect is the idea that individuals compare
their consumption habits with others. Thus, what is desirable is not only a
function of the intrinsic attributes of a material possession or a reflection
of one's true preferences, but also a function of what significant and similar
others possess. What this shows is the interdependence of consumer preferences,
lending evidence to the important role of social comparison as a source of information
for consumers. Through social comparisons, consumers learn what is valuable and
desirable.
With regard to social
standing, there is evidence suggesting that individuals compare their own
material possessions with those owned by significant others to ascertain their
social status ( Saunders, 2001 ). Humans are equipped
to notice and pay close attention to social status and rank ( Hill & Buss, 2008 ). It follows then that
if material possessions are reliable indicators of social rank, then
individuals would have the tendency to compare what they have in relation to
others.
Even though the proposed
relationship between the endorsement of material values and social comparison
appears to be consistent across cultures, this relationship might be
particularly strong for cultures where the self is defined interdependently,
collectivistic cultures. For example in the Chinese culture, social comparison
of material possessions serves the purpose of positioning individuals in the
social hierarchy, being therefore encouraged ( Wong & Ahuvia, 1998 ). Similarly, a study in Japan
found that social comparison was positively related with a higher desire for
more possessions and higher consumption intentions ( Ogden & Venkat, 2001 ). These two cross-cultural
investigations, however, had some limitations. Whereas Chine and Japanese cultures
can be considered collectivistic in nature, they represent a type of
collectivism based on humility, referred to as Confucian-based collectivism.
Conversely, Latin-American cultures such as Brazil or Mexico endorse a type of
collectivism based on honor ( Uskul, Oyserman,
& Schwarz, 2010 ; Zampieri et al., 2012 ). Thus, investigations
with participants from collectivistic cultures of honor would allow us to
corroborate the importance of social comparisons in consumption related values
such as materialism. There is evidence already about the importance of
significant others in collectivistic cultures of honor ( Diaz-Guerrero, 2008 ). To test the role of
social comparison as an antecedent of material values, the following hypothesis
is formulated:
- 1.
Social
comparison orientation would have a positive effect on materialism.
As
mentioned earlier, the theory of materialistic values ( Kasser, 2002 ) also proposes the
lack of satisfaction of basic psychological needs as an antecedent of
materialism. Self-determination theory suggests that humans share three
psychological needs that should be satisfied in order to have optimal
functioning ( Ryan & Deci, 2002 ). One of these needs, the need
for autonomy, has been examined and found to be an antecedent of material
values ( Kasser, Ryan, Couchman, & Sheldon, 2004 ). Specifically, the
theory of material values suggests that when individuals feel that they do not
have an environment supportive of their autonomy, they might orient toward
material values in order to compensate for the lack of autonomy support ( Kasser, 2002 ). Consequently, low
levels of autonomy support should increase the likelihood of endorsing material
values. The following hypothesis is formulated:
- 2.
Autonomy
support would have a negative effect on materialism.
In
sum, we attempt to test the role of social comparison orientation as an
antecedent of materialism (study 1) and the role of social comparison
orientation and autonomy support as antecedents of materialism (study 2). We
now turn our attention to some of the possible consequences of holding material
values.
Consequences: affective,
cognitive and behavioral
The importance of values
lies in that individual differences in the endorsement materialism are expected
to lead to different cognitive, affective and behavioral outcomes ( Kasser, 2002 ). For example,
materialistic values affect how individuals related to possessions, work and
spend money on themselves and friends ( Deckop, Jurkiewicz,
& Giancalone, 2010 ; Roberts, 2011 ). Among the different
outcomes that materialism is likely to influence, consumers’ subjective
well-being is particularly relevant.
Researchers have suggested
that subjective well-being has three components: cognitive assessment of life
satisfaction, positive, and negative affect ( Diener, 1984 ). Across different
investigations, materialism seems to exert a negative effect on subjective
well-being ( Burroughs & Rindfleisch, 2002; Dittmar, Bond, Hurst, & Kasser, 2014 ). However, in our opinion, many of
these investigations have had limitations. First, several investigations have
not conceptualized well-being as a three component psychological construct ( Burroughs & Rindfleisch, 2002 ). That is, investigations have
focused mainly on assessing the impact of materialism on judgments of general
and domain specific satisfaction, paying less attention to the influence of
materialism on positive and negative affect. Second, most investigations have
been conducted with participants from individualistic cultures ( Burroughs & Rindfleisch, 2002 ), limiting our ability to
generalize results to other cultures. For example in collectivistic cultures of
honor such as Mexico, reputation and the opinion of others are two important
factors that could potentially influence materialism and its effect on
subjective well-being (see Podoshen, Li & Zhang, 2010; Workman & Lee,
2010; Xiao & Kim, 2009 for recent exceptions examining materialism
among members from Confucian based collective cultures such as China and
Korea). The following hypotheses are formulated:
·
3a. Materialism would have a negative effect on satisfaction with life.
·
3b. Materialism would have a negative effect on positive affect.
·
3c. Materialism would have a positive effect on negative affect.
Our current economic and
social systems encourage the acquisition of material possessions ( Kasser, 2002 ). Yet, most people
have limited financial resources. Thus, the desire toward acquiring material
possessions should have multiple consequences in different areas of people's
lives. There is evidence suggesting that material values are likely to
influence different consumption related outcomes ( Deckop, Jurkiewicz,
& Giancalone, 2010 ). However, we suggest
that their impact goes beyond traditional consumption variables and include
relevant outcomes such as a preference for particular jobs that provide more
monetary resources.
Some investigations have
shown that highly materialistic individuals report feelings of external
pressure to achieve goals consistent with materialistic values ( Sheldon & Kasser, 2001 ), including the desire to make
more money ( Lemrová, Reiterová,
Fatenová, Lemr, & Tang,
2014 ; Srivastava, Locke,
& Borrol, 2001 ) and the idea that
financial success should be a fundamental aspiration in their value structure (
Grouzet et al., 2005 ). However, these
investigations did not actually examine a tradeoff that is inherent in some
financial decisions: A desire to make more money at the expense of making some
sacrifices. In other words, it is likely that most people would want to earn
more money, yet it is the tradeoff between more money and some sacrifices what
we consider to be of special interest. In order to assess this, we study if
material values are capable of predicting preferences for jobs requiring longer
hours or with less diversity of tasks in order to make more money and test the
following hypothesis:
·
4. Materialism would increase the probability of choosing jobs with higher
salaries but longer working days or more routine tasks.
Implications
The examination of the
antecedents and consequences of material values might have implications for
marketing and human resources. First, marketing makes the assumption that the satisfaction
of consumer needs is the driving force behind the development of products and
services ( Kotler & Keller,
2013 ). Yet as suggested by our literature review, when individuals
emphasize the acquisition of material values, they might end up being less
satisfied, which represents an interesting paradox for marketers. Second, human
resources departments seek to hire the best possible employees for their
organizations. Knowing that material values might potentially influence the
attractiveness of different job offers, human resources personnel might be able
to infer the job orientation of individuals, seeking more extrinsic or
intrinsic job outcomes ( Hall & Las Heras,
2012) and its possible consequences.
In sum, the purpose of
the present investigation is twofold. First, we test the role of social
comparison orientation and autonomy support as antecedents of material values.
Second, we examine the affective, positive and negative affect, cognitive, life
satisfaction, and behavioral consequences, choosing job offers with higher
(lower) salaries at the expense of longer (shorter) working days and routine
(diverse) tasks, of holding material values.
Overview of studies
Two studies were
conducted test some antecedents and consequences of holding material values.
Study 1 examined the role of social comparison orientation as an antecedent of
material values and the influence of material values on satisfaction with life,
positive and negative affect, and job preferences. Study 2 examined the role of
social comparison orientation and autonomy support as antecedents of material
values and also the influence of material values on positive and negative
affect.
Participants and
procedure study 1
Participants were 386
(247 females and 139 males) college students from two private universities in
the Mexico City area (Universidad Anáhuac) and Puebla
(Universidad Popular Autónoma del Estado de Puebla).
The mean age was 22.47 (SD = 2.45).
Students received extra credit for their participation.
Participants completed the
questionnaires in small groups of two or three. The questionnaires took between
15 and 20 min to complete. All participants were thanked and debriefed after
their participation. The present investigation obtained the approval from the
institutional human subjects committee.
Measures
The Satisfaction with
Life Scale (SWLS) ( Diener, Emmons, Larsen,
& Griffin, 1985 ). The SWLS is a five-item questionnaire designed to
measure satisfaction with life. The questionnaire uses a Likert-type scale and
total scores range from 5 to 35. The scale showed adequate psychometric
properties for scientific research (e.g., significant loadings and a coefficient
of internal consistency of .84) and previous research has also shown acceptable
properties with Spanish speaking participants ( Vazquez, Duque, & Hervas, 2013 ).
The Scale of Positive
and Negative Experience (SPANE) ( Diener et al., 2009 ). The SPANE is a
twelve-item questionnaire that includes six items to assess positive feelings
and six items to assess negative feelings. Each item is scored on a scale
ranging from 1 to 5, where 1 represents “very rarely or never” and 5 represents
“very often or always.” The positive and negative scales are scored separately.
The scale showed adequate psychometric properties for scientific research
(e.g., significant loadings and coefficients of internal consistency above
.67). Similar scales have been validated with Spanish speaking participants
(e.g., Moriondo, De Palma, Medrano,
& Murillo, 2012 ).
The Material Value Scale
(MVS) ( Richins, 2004 ). We used the
abbreviated nine-item measure of materialistic values. Each item is scored on a
scale ranging from 1 to 5, where 1 represents “strongly disagree” and 5
represents “strongly agree.” The scale possesses good psychometric properties,
especially when it is used to measure materialism at the general level. The
scale showed adequate psychometric properties for scientific research (e.g.,
significant loadings and a coefficient of internal consistency of .78) and has
shown adequate psychometric properties with Spanish speaking participants as
well ( Puente-Díaz & Cavazos-Arroyo, 2015 ).
The Iowa-Netherlands
Comparison Orientation Measure (INCOM) ( Gibbons & Buunk, 1999 ). The INCOM is an eleven-item
questionnaire designed to assess individual differences in social comparison
orientation. Each item is scored on a scale ranging from 1 to 5, where 1
represents “strongly disagree” and 5 represents “strongly agree.” Higher scores
present higher levels of social comparison orientation. The scale showed
adequate psychometric properties for scientific research (e.g., significant
loadings and a coefficient of internal consistency of .79) and has been
validated with Spanish speaking participants (e.g., Buunk, Belmonte, Peiró, Zurriaga, & Gibbons,
2005 ).
Decision making task. We
presented participants with one of two decision making tasks. In one task,
participants had to choose between a job with a monthly salary of 1300 American
dollars and routine tasks vs. 1000 American dollars and diverse tasks.
Similarly, the second decision making tasks involved two options: a job with a
monthly salary of 1300 American dollars and 10-h working days vs. 1000 American
dollars and 8-h working days. Participants were only exposed, randomly, to one
of the two tasks. Both tasks involved a tradeoff between making more money and
either having a less challenging or boring job (routine task) or working longer
hours. This decision making task was designed to test whether material values
influence choices that are somewhat related but at the same time independent of
consumption.
Results study 1
We used Structural
Equation Modeling (SEM) with LISREL 9.1 to test our hypotheses. In order to
assess the robustness of the model, we used a combination of absolute and
incremental fit index. Thus, we report the χ 2 , Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and the Incremental Fit
Index (IFI) for each of the analysis conducted. We used the cutoff scores of
RMSEA < .08 and IFI > .90 as
the minimum acceptable levels of model fit ( West, Taylor, &
Wu, 2012).
We first examined the
measurement model for the latent variables materialism, social comparison,
satisfaction with life, and positive and negative affect. Results showed that
the model fit was adequate χ 2 = 1137.16,
p = .00
(df = 617), RMSEA = .05 and IFI = .94. Examination of individual parameters
revealed that all factor loadings were significant and in the expected
direction. Thus, we decided to keep this measurement model and proceeded to
test the structural model.
Results showed that the
model fit was adequate χ 2 = 1207.57, p = .00 (df = 622), RMSEA = .05 and IFI = .93. Examination of the individual parameters
revealed that social comparison had significant positive effect on materialism,
γ = .43, p
< .05. Thus, hypothesis 1 was supported.
Regarding the consequences of holding material values, we found a significant
negative effect of materialism on satisfaction with life and positive affect, β = −.16, p
< .05; β = −.23, p
< .05, respectively, while controlling for
the effect of positive affect on satisfaction with life, supporting hypothesis
3a and 3b. Lastly, results also showed a significant positive effect of
materialism on negative affect, β = .32, p < .05,
supporting hypothesis 3c (see Fig. 1 for the complete
model). Squared multiple correlations for the endogenous variables were,
respectively, materialism (0.18), satisfaction with life (0.33), positive
affect (0.05), and negative affect (0.10).
Since we were interested
in examining the effect of materialistic values and social comparison on the
probability of choosing a job with a higher salary and longer hours or higher
salary and less diverse activities, we conducted two logistic regression
analyses. Job offers with lower salaries were coded as 0 and offers with higher
salaries were coded as 1. Consequently, the logistic regression analyses
allowed us to examine the influence of materialism on the probability of
choosing job offers with higher salaries but longer hours or less diverse
activities. Descriptive statistics showed that 44% of participants chose the
job with a higher salary and routine activities, whereas 60% selected the job
with higher salary or longer hours (10 vs. 8). The results showed that
materialism had positive effect on the probability of choosing both job offers
with a higher salary and more routine activities and longer hours ( β = .56, p
< .05, exp β = 1.75; β = .66, p
< .05, exp β = 1.94,
respectively). The effects of social comparison were not significant in any of
the two analyses ( β = .13, p
> .05; β = −.04, p
> .05, respectively). Thus, hypothesis 4
was supported.
Brief discussion study 1
Our results showed that
social comparison had a significant, positive effect on materialism. Results
also showed that materialism influenced affective, cognitive and behavioral outcomes.
The significant effect of social comparison on materialism was encouraging, yet
we believe it was necessary to conduct an additional study to further validate
the findings obtained in study 1 and analyze autonomy support as another
antecedent of materialism.
Participants and
procedure study 2
Participants were 339
(194 females and 145 males) college students from two private universities in
the Mexico City area (Universidad Anáhuac) and Puebla
(Universidad Popular Autónoma del Estado de Puebla).
The mean age was 21.54 (SD = 2.61).
Students received extra credit for their participation.
Participants completed
the questionnaires in small groups of two or three. The questionnaires took
between 15 and 20 min to complete. All participants were thanked and debriefed
after their participation. The present investigation obtained the approval from
the institutional human subjects committee.
Measures
For study 2, the same
questionnaires, as in study 1, measuring materialism, social comparison, and
positive and negative affect were used. Consistent with study 1, the
coefficients of internal consistency were acceptable: materialism (.79), social
comparison (.79), positive affect (.83) and negative affect (.71). The only new
questionnaire was:
General Need Satisfaction
Scale (Gagné, 2003 ). This scale was
adapted based on Self-Determination Theory by Gagné (2003) and uses 21 items to
measure need satisfaction. Respondents indicated on a scale from 1 (not true at
all) to 7 (definitely true) the extent to which the psychological needs of
autonomy, relatedness, and competence are generally satisfied in their lives.
For the purpose of our investigation, we only measured autonomy support. The
scale showed adequate psychometric properties (e.g., significant loadings and a
coefficient of internal consistency of .65) and has been validated with Spanish
speaking participants (e.g., Domínguez, Martín, Martín-Albo, Núñez, & León, 2010 ).
Results study 2
We first examined the
measurement model for the latent variables social comparison, materialism,
autonomy support and positive and negative affect. Results showed that the
model fit was adequate χ 2 = 1593.48, p = .00 (df = 645), RMSEA = .07 and IFI = .91. Examination of individual parameters
revealed that all factor loadings were significant and in the expected
direction. Thus, we decided to keep this measurement model and proceed to test
the structural model.
Results showed that the
model fit was barely adequate χ 2 = 1667.99,
p = .00
(df = 649), RMSEA = .07 and IFI = .90. Examination of the individual parameters
showed that autonomy support did not have a significant effect on materialism, γ = −.02, p
> .05, failing to support hypothesis 3a.
We decided to drop the non-significant effect of autonomy support and conduct
the analysis again since the overall fit of the model could be improved.
Results showed that the model fit was better χ 2 = 932.67, p = .00 (df = 420), RMSEA = .06 and IFI = .92. Consistent with study 1, examination of the
individual parameters revealed a significant positive affect of social
comparison on materialism, γ = .47, p < .05.
Similarly, the effects of materialism on positive and negative affect were also
significant and in the expected direction, β = −.31, p
< .05; β = .32, p
< .05, respectively (see Fig. 2 for the complete
model).
Brief discussion study 2
Study 2 further
validated the importance of social comparison as an antecedent of materialism.
Results also showed that autonomy support did not predict the endorsement of material
values. Lastly, materialism had a significant effect on positive and negative
affect.
General discussion
In this investigation,
we examined the role of social comparison and autonomy support as antecedents
of material values and also the affective, cognitive and behavioral
consequences of holding this type of values. We found strong support for the
role of social comparison as an antecedent of materialism. Social comparison
was a robust predictor of the adoption of material values, even stronger than
the influence of autonomy support. Why would social comparison be such as
important component in the adoption of material values? In order to answer this
important question, we need to explain what characterizes the evaluation of
material possessions.
Social comparisons and
material values
We compare ourselves
with others because it represents a natural way of knowing more about
ourselves. Yet a more intriguing question is why social comparison is a key
antecedent of materialism. In order to explain this, we have to rely on the
difference between the evaluability of experiences vs. material possessions.
Evaluability theory suggests that certain attributes or outcomes are easier to
evaluate by humans than others ( Hsee, Hastie, & Che, 2008 ). The theory suggests that certain
outcomes are high in evaluability because humans have innate biological scales
to judge their desirability. Conversely, other outcomes are low in evaluability
because humans do not have innate, but rather socially learned scales to judge
their desirability. The desirability of outcomes with low evaluability does not
depend only on the characteristics of the outcomes but also on external,
socially learned reference points. Examples of outcomes with high evaluability
are temperature, stress from work, and social isolation, among others. Income,
wealth, and material possessions are examples of outcomes with low
evaluability. The desirability of one's material possessions is not only a
function of the absolute value they have, but also of what important others
possess. It is more comparative in nature. Thus, social comparisons become a
driving force to decide and estimate what is desirable to possess. Humans are
not naturally equipped to evaluate material possessions, thus the evaluation
comes from comparing what we have in relation to others and social comparisons
might be particularly relevant in cultures that define the self interdependently
such as Mexico ( Diaz-Guerrero, 2008).
Our results also showed that
the adoption of material values was related to lower levels of positive affect
and satisfaction with life and to higher levels of negative affect. Our results
are consistent with a wide range of investigation showing the negative
consequences of holding material values ( Burroughs & Rindfleisch, 2002; Kasser, 2002 ). However, if, as some
authors have suggested, the adoption of material values is so widespread ( Bauman, 2007; Kasser, 2002 ), what are some of the advantages
of holding material values? If it does not lead to higher levels of subjective
well-being, what does it lead to?
The possible advantages
of endorsing material values
If materialism is
related to lower levels of life satisfaction and positive affect, and higher
levels of negative affect, what are some of the advantages of materialism? The
answer might come from the conceptualization of situations from evolutionary
psychology ( Buss, 2009 ). According to evolutionary
psychology, we are designed to pay close attention and act on situations that
facilitate or hinder accomplishing one of the main objectives of human life,
which is to pass one's genes successfully. In order to accomplish so, people
need to be able to choose an appropriate mate. Thus, finding ways to attract a
desirable mate is a situation that humans are designed to pay attention to and
act on it. The chances of having access to an optimal mate increase
substantially if one ranks higher in terms of social status, and material
possessions are a way of communicating social rank. It follows then that having
material possessions might be a way to present oneself as an “attractive”
candidate to potential mates by communicating high social status ( Rose & Conlon,
2009).
As suggested earlier,
materialism should not only influence consumption related variables, but also
behaviors that would facilitate reaching important goals. Our results showed
that people with higher levels of materialism were more likely to choose job
offers that involved working longer hours and doing more routine activities in
order to make more money. Thus, when face with the tradeoff between having more
free time or a more exciting job, people with high levels of materialism choose
having the chance to make more money. This decision should have a small effect
on their well-being since research indicates that income has a small effect on
well-being ( Rojas, 2009 ), but should
facilitate the acquisition of material possessions, which ironically, does not
lead to higher levels of subjective well-being.
Our findings, then,
might have implications for marketing and human resources. Emphasizing the
importance of acquiring material goods was related to lower levels of
satisfaction with life and positive affect and higher levels of negative
affect. One line research suggests that consumers should be encourage to buy
experiences rather than material goods since experiences lead to higher levels
of satisfaction ( Dunn, Gilbert, &
Wilson, 2011 ). Regarding the implications for human resources, research indicates
that the quality of reasons for having a job influences job satisfaction.
Employees who have more intrinsic reasons for having a job report higher levels
of satisfaction than employees who have more extrinsic reasons such as wanting
to make more money ( Hall & Las Heras,
2012 ). Our results showed that participants with higher levels of
materialism were more likely to choose jobs with higher salaries but with
longer working hours or more routine tasks; jobs likely to lead to lower levels
of job satisfaction.
Limitations and future
directions
Our investigation had
several limitations. First, we used a correlational design and collected data
at one point in time only. Thus, even though we predicted some variables to act
as antecedents and others as consequences of material values, we could not
actually test for this. Future research should put more emphasis on using
longitudinal designs to determine how the tendency to compare one with others
increases the importance of adopting material values and how the endorsement of
material values might lead to experiencing less positive affect and
satisfaction with life. Similarly, longitudinal designs would allow us to
examine how material values might influence other important variables such as
debt acquisition across time.
Another limitation is
that we used a sample of convenience: college students. Students from a private
university do not represent well the general population of Mexico. Mexico is a
country where income inequalities are considerable ( Gasparini & Lustig, 2011 ). Thus, future research should
assess whether the model of antecedents and consequences of material values
tested here would generalize to other segments of the Mexican population. Last,
another limitation is that the theoretical models used to guide our
investigation were limited in scope. In other words, it would have been ideal
to use a model that fully postulated the antecedents and consequences of
material values and how cultural variables might have played a role. Hence, we
hope that our results can provide empirical evidence for future theory or model
development.
In sum, we were able to
establish the importance of social comparisons as an antecedent of material
values. Given the common occurrence of social comparisons, it is not surprising
to see the widespread of materialism in today's society ( Bauman, 2007 ). We were also able to
establish some of the consequences of holding material values. Materialism was
related to lower levels of life satisfaction and positive affect and to higher
levels of negative affect. Similarly, materialism also influenced choices of
job offers with more hours and more routines activities in order to make more
money, lending evidence to the importance of values across different life
domains.
Bauman,
Z. (2007). Consuming life. Hoboken,
NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Burroughs,
J. E., & Rindfleisch, A. (2002). Materialism and
well-being: A conflicting values perspective. Journal
of Consumer Research, 29(3), 348–370.
https://doi.org/10.1086/344429
Buunk, B. P., Belmonte, J., Peiró,
J. M., Zurriaga, R., & Gibbons, F. X. (2005). Diferencias
individuales en la comparación social: Propiedades de la escala espa˜nola de orientación hacia la comparación social. Revista Latinoamericana
de Psicología, 37, 561–581.
Buss, D.
M. (2009). An evolutionary formulation of person–situation interactions. Journal
of Research in Personality, 43(2), 241–242.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.12.019
Deckop, J. R., Jurkiewicz,
C. L., & Giacalone, R. A. (2010). Effects of materialism
on work-related personal well-being. Human
Relations, 63(7), 1007–1030. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018726709353953
Diaz-Guerrero,
R. (2008). Psicología del Mexicano: Descubrimiento de la etnopsicología.
México, Ciudad de México: Editorial Trillas.
Diener, E.
(1984). Subjective well-being.
Psychological Bulletin, 95(1), 542–575.
https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.95.3.542
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., &
Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal
of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901
13
Diener, E., Wirtz, D.,
Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi.F D., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Diener,
R. (2009). New measures of well-being: Flourishing and positive and negative
feelings. Social Indicators Research, 39(1), 247–266.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-009-9493-y
Dittmar, H., Bond, R., Hurst, M., & Kasser, T. (2014). The relationship between materialism and
personal well-being: A meta-analysis. Journal
of Personality and Social psychology, 107(5), 879.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037409
Domínguez,
E., Martín, P., Martín-Albo, J., Nú˜nez, J. L., &
León, J. (2010). Translation and Validation of the Spanish Version
of the “Échelle de Satisfaction des Besoins Psychologiques” in the
Sports Context. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 13, 1010–1020.
https://doi.org/10.1017/s1138741600002651
Duesenberry, J. (1949). Income,
savings and the theory of consumer behavior.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Dunn, E.
W., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2011). If money doesn’t make you
happy, then you probably aren’t spending it right. Journal
of Consumer Psychology, 21, 115–125.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2011.02.002
Festinger, L. (1954). A
theory of social comparison processes. Human
Relations, 7, 117–140.
Gagné, M. (2003). The
role of autonomy support and autonomy orientation in prosocial behavior
engagement. Motivation and Emotion, 27(3),
199–223.
Gasparini, L., & Lustig,
N. (2011). The rise and fall of income inequality in Latin America. In J. A. Ocampo, & J. Ros (Eds.), Oxford
Handbook of Latin American Economics (pp.
691–714). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Gibbons,
F. X., & Buunk, B. P. (1999). Individual
differences in social comparison: Development of a scale of social comparison
orientation. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 76(1), 129–142.
https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.76.1.129
Grougiou, V., & Moschis,
G. P. (2015). Antecedents of young adults’ materialistic values. Journal
of Consumer Behaviour,14(2), 115–126.
https://doi.org/10.1002/cb.1505
Grouzet, F., Kasser,
T., Ahuvia, A., Dols, J.,
Kim, Y., Lau, S., Ryan, R., Saunders, S., Schmuck, P., & Sheldon, K. M.
(2005). The structure of goal contents across 15 cultures. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(5), 800–816.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.5.800
Hall, D.
T., & Las Heras, M. (2012). Personal growth through
career work. In K. S. Cameron, & G. M. Spreitzer
(Eds.), The Oxford handbook of positive organizational scholarship (pp.
507–518). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Hill, S.
E., & Buss, D. M. (2008). The evolutionary psychology of envy. In
R. H. Smith (Ed.), Envy: Theory and research (pp. 60–70).
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Hsee, C. K., Hastie, R., & Chen, J.
(2008). Hedonomics:
Bridging decision research with happiness research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(3),
224–243, tps://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00076.x
Kasser, T. (2002). The high price of
materialism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kasser, T., Ryan, R. M., Couchman, C., &
Sheldon, K. M. (2004). Materialistic values:
Their causes and consequences. In T. Kasser, & A.
Kanner (Eds.), Psychology and consumer culture:
The struggle for a good life in a materialistic
world (pp. 11–28).
Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.
Kotler, P., & Keller, K. L. (2013). Dirección de Marketing (13th ed.). México,
DF: Pearson.
Lemrová, S., Reiterová, E., Fatˇenová, R., Lemr, K., & Tang, T. L. P.
(2014). Money is power: Monetary intelligence—Love
of money and temptation of materialism among Czech University students. Journal
of Business Ethics, 125(2), 329–348. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-013-1915-8
Moriondo, M., Palma, P., Medrano, L. A., & Murillo, P. (2012). Adaptación de la Escala de Afectividad Positiva y Negativa (PANAS) a la
población de adultos de la ciudad de Córdoba: análisis psicométricos
preliminares. Universitas Psychologica, 11(1), 187–196.
Mussweiler, T. (2007). Assimilation and
contrast as comparison effects: A selective accessibility model. In D. A. Stapel, & J. Suls (Eds.), Assimilation
and contrast in social psychology (pp. 165–185). New York: Psychology
Press.
Ogden, H. J., & Venkat,
R. (2001). Social comparison and possessions: Japan vs Canada. Asia Pacific
Journal of Marketing and Logistics, 13(2), 72–84. https://doi.org/10.1108/13555850110764775
Pepper, M., Jackson, T., & Uzzell,
D. (2009). An examination of the values that motivate socially conscious and frugal
consumer behaviours. International Journal of
Consumer Studies, 33(2), 126–136. https://doi.org/10.1111/
j.1470-6431.2009.00753.x
Podoshen, J. F., Li, L., & Zhang, J. (2010). Materialism and conspicuous
consumption in China: A cross-cultural examination. International Journal of
Consumer Studies, 35(1), 17–25. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1470-6431.2010.00930.x
Puente-Díaz, R., & Cavazos-Arroyo, J. (2015). The influence of personality dimensions on material
and frugal values. Anales de Psicología, 31, 37–44. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.31.1.167401
Richins, M. L. (2004). The material values scale: Measurement properties and
development of a short form. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(1),
209–219. https://doi.org/10.1086/383436
Richins, M. L., & Dawson, S. (1992). A consumer values orientation for
materialism and its measurement: Scale development and validation. Journal
of Consumer Research, 19(3), 303–316. https://doi.org/10.1086/209304
Roberts, J. A. (2011). Shiny objects: Why we spend money we don’t have in search of happiness
we can’t buy. New York: Harper One.
Rojas, M. (2009). Economía de la felicidad: Hallazgos
relevantes respecto al ingreso y el bienestar. El Trimestre Económico, 76(July–September), 537–573.
Rose, P., & Colon, K. (2009). Materialism: An evolutionary perspective. In F. Saito
(Ed.), Consumer behavior (pp.185–196). New York: Nova Science
Publishers.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2002). An overview of self-determination theory: An organismic-dialectical
perspective. In E. L. Deci, & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook
of self-determination research (pp. 3–33). New York: The University of
Rochester Press.
Saunders, S. (2001). Fromm’s marketing character and Rokeach values. Social Behavior and Personality, 29(2),
191–196. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2001.29.2.191
Schwartz, S. H. (2005). Basic human values: Their content and structure across countries. In A. Tamayo, & J. B. Porto (Eds.), Valores e comportamento
nas organiza¸cões (pp.
21–55). Petrópolis, Brazil: Vozes.
Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser,
T. (2001). Getting older, getting better? Personal strivings and personality
development across the life-course. Developmental Psychology, 37(4),
491–501. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.37.4.491
Srivastava, A., Locke, E. A., & Bortol, K. M. (2001). Money and subjective well-being: It’s
not the money, it’s the motives. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 80(6), 959–971. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.80.6.959
Uskul, A. K., Oyserman, D., & Schwarz, N.
(2010). Cultural emphasis on honor, modesty or
self enhancement: Implications for the survey response process. In J. Harkness,
J. Harkness, et al. (Eds.), Survey methods in multinational,
multiregional, and multicultural contexts (pp. 191–201). New York: Wiley.
Vazquez, C., Duque, A., & Hervas,
G. (2013). Satisfaction with life scale in a representative sample of Spanish
adults: Validation and normative data. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 16,
E82. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2013.82
West, S. G., Taylor, A. B., & Wu, W. (2012). Model fit and model selection in structural equation
modeling. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Handbook of structural equation modeling (pp.
209–231). New York: Guilford Press.
Wong, N. Y., & Ahuvia,
A. C. (1998). Personal taste and family face: Luxury
consumption in Confucian and Western societies. Psychology & Marketing,
15(5), 423–441, https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1520-6793(199808)15:5<423::aidmar2>
3.0.co;2-9
Workman, J. E., & Lee, S. H. (2010). Materialism,
fashion consumers and gender: A cross-cultural study. International Journal of Consumer
Studies, 35(1), 50–57. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1470-6431.2010.00935.x
Xiao, G., & Kim, J. O. (2009). The investigation
of Chinese consumer values, life satisfaction, and consumption behaviors. Psychology
& Marketing, 26(7), 610–624. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.20291
Zampieri, M., Flores, L., Beuron, T. A., Aitta, L., de Moura, A., & Lutz, C. (2012).
Relac¸ão entre materialismo e estilo de consumo: homens e mulheres com comportamento
dispare? Contaduría y Administración, 57(1), 185–214.
https://doi.org/10.5585/remark.v11i3.2292
Peer Review under the responsibility
of Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México.
Métricas de artículo
Metrics powered by PLOS ALM
Enlaces refback
- No hay ningún enlace refback.

Este obra está bajo una licencia de Creative Commons Reconocimiento-NoComercial-CompartirIgual 4.0 Internacional.
CONTADURÍA Y ADMINISTRACIÓN, año 70, 2025, es una publicación trimestral editada por la Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Colonia Ciudad Universitaria, Delegación Coyoacán, C.P. 04510, México, Ciudad de México, a través de la División de Investigación de la Facultad de Contaduría y Administración - UNAM, Circuito Exterior, s/n, Colonia Ciudad Universitaria, Delegación Coyoacán, C.P. 04510, México, Ciudad de México., Tels. (55) 56 22 84 57, (55) 56 22 84 58 Ext. 144 y (55) 56 22 84 94, http://www.cya.unam.mx, correo electrónico: revista_cya@fca.unam.mx, Editor responsable: José Alberto García Narváez, Reserva de Derechos al Uso Exclusivo No. 04-2016-071316434900-203, otorgada por el Instituto Nacional del Derecho de Autor, ISSN 2448-8410, Responsable de la última actualización de este Número, División de Investigación de la Facultad de Contaduría y Administración-UNAM, José Alberto García Narváez, Circuito Exterior, s/n, Colonia Ciudad Universitaria, Delegación Coyoacán, C.P. 04510, México, Cd., Mx., fecha de última modificación, 29 de enero de 2025.
Las opiniones expresadas por los autores no necesariamente reflejan la postura del editor de la publicación. Se autoriza la reproducción total o parcial de los textos aquí publicados siempre y cuando se cite la fuente completa y la dirección electrónica de la publicación.
Contaduría y Administración by División de Investigación de la Facultad de Contaduría y Administración is licensed under a Creative Commons Reconocimiento- 4.0 Internacional.
Creado a partir de la obra en http://www.cya.unam.mx.
Correo electrónico: revista_cya@fca.unam.mx
ISSN: 0186-1042 (Print) 2448-8410 (Online)